Trickery in Nature
In
1862, the English explorer-naturalist Henry Bates returned from a decade-long
exploration of the Brazilian Amazon rainforest and, after examining almost one
hundred species of butterflies, he reported his unusual findings. Of particular
interest were two distant families bearing a close resemblance: one family,
Heliconidae, is brightly colored and unpalatable to birds; the other family,
Pieridae, is also colorful but palatable to predators. Bates surmised that the
coloration of the unpalatable species proclaimed a warning to potential
predators of their poor taste based on the birds’ previous experience. He also
noted that some palatable butterflies bearing a close resemblance to
unpalatable ones were avoided by predators. This has been called Batesian
mimicry.
Other
favorable evolutionary adaptations involving mimicry have been observed in
nature. The mimic gains an advantage from its resemblance to another organism
(the “model”) and a third party mistakes the mimic for the model, such as the
harmless colubrid snake that mimics the characteristic “hood” of the Indian
cobra’s threat display. The third party may be a potential predator or a
potential prey of the mimic. Following observations by Bates, other examples
have been seen in plants and animals, and in some cases, plants mimic animals
and vice versa. The most common examples of mimicry are based on appearance,
but sound, smell, and behavior are also imitated.
The
German zoologist Fritz Müller noted in 1878 that two unrelated and unpalatable
species of butterflies had similar color patterns, and each had adequate
defense mechanisms—an apparent exception to Batesian mimicry. But once a
predator had learned to avoid a butterfly with one color pattern, it would
avoid all other species with a similar pattern (Müllerian mimicry). Animals
exhibiting aposematism transmit a warning signal (pronounced color, sound,
odor, or taste) to potential predators that they possess a secondary and more
potent defense mechanism—as do the brightly colored poison dart frog or skunk.
Aggressive mimicry can be used to escape the detection of potential prey;
similarly, in an example of inter-sexual mimicry, the male cuttlefish
camouflages itself as a female in order to escape the detection of other males
and get closer to females. And some plants, including orchids, mimic female
bees and wasps to attract males, which results in pollination of the plant.
No comments:
Post a Comment